Читаем Мозг – повелитель времени полностью

— Lebedev, M. A., O’Doherty, J. E., Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2008). Decoding of temporal intervals from cortical ensemble activity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99, 166–186.

— Lee, T. P., Buonomano, D. V. (2012). Unsupervised formation of vocalization-sensitive neurons: a cortical model based on short-term and homeostatic plasticity. Neural Computation, 24, 2579–2603.

— Lehiste, I. (1960). An acoustic — phonetic study of internal open juncture. Phonetica, 5 (suppl. 1), 5–54.

— Lehiste, I., Olive, J. P., Streeter, L. A. (1976). Role of duration in disambiguating syntactically ambiguous sentences. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 60, 1199–1202.

— Leon, M. I., Shadlen, M. N. (2003). Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron, 38, 317–327.

— Levine, R. (1996). The geography of time. New York: Basic Books.

— Levy, W. B., Steward, O. (1983). Temporal contiguity requirements for long-term associative potentiation/depression in the hippocampus. Neuroscience, 8, 791–797.

— Lewis, P. A., Miall, R. C., Daan, S., Kacelnik, A. (2003). Interval timing in mice does not rely upon the circadian pacemaker. Neuroscience Letters, 348, 131–134.

— Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106 (Pt. 3), 623–642.

— Lieving, L. M., Lane, S. D., Cherek, D. R., Tcheremissine, O. V. (2006). Effects of marijuana on temporal discriminations in humans. Behavioral Pharmacology, 17, 173–183.

— Livesey, A. C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T. (2007). Time perception: Manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands. Neuropsychologia, 45, 321–331.

— Lockwood, M. (2005). The labyrinth of time: Introducing the universe. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

— Loftus, E. F. (1996). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

— Loftus, E. F., Schooler, J. W., Boone, S. M., Kline, D. (1987). Time went by so slowly: overestimation of event duration by males and females. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1, 3–13.

— Loh, D. H., Navarro, J., Hagopian, A., Wang, L. M., Deboer, T., Colwell, C. S. (2010). Rapid changes in the light/dark cycle disrupt memory of conditioned fear in mice. PLoS ONE, 5, e12546.

— Lombardi, M. A. (2002). Fundamentals of time and frequency. In: Mechanotronics handbook (Bishop, RH, ed.). New York: CRC Press.

— Long, M. A., Fee, M. S. (2008). Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature, 456, 189–194.

— Long, M. A., Jin, D. Z., Fee, M. S. (2010). Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation. Nature, 468, 394–399.

— Maass, W., Natschläger, T., Markram, H. (2002). Real-time computing without stable states: A new framework for neural computation based on perturbations. Neural Computation, 14, 2531–2560.

— MacDonald, C. J., Lepage, K. Q., Eden, U. T., Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Hippocampal “time cells” bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron, 71, 737–749.

— MacDonald, C. J., Carrow, S., Place, R., Eichenbaum, H. (2013). Distinct hippocampal time cell sequences represent odor memories in immobilized rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 14607–14616.

— MacKillop, J., Amlung, M. T., Few, L. R., Ray, L. A., Sweet, L. H., Munafo, M. R. (2011). Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl.), 216, 305–321.

— Mante, V., Sussillo, D., Shenoy, K. V., Newsome, W. T. (2013). Context-dependent computation by recurrent dynamics in prefrontal cortex. Nature, 503, 78–84.

— Markram, H., Lubke, J., Frotscher, M., Sakmann, B. (1997). Regulation of synaptic efficacy by coincidence of postsynaptic APs and EPSPs. Science, 275, 213–215.

— Martin, F. H., Garfield, J. (2006). Combined effects of alcohol and caffeine on the late components of the event-related potential and on reaction time. Biological Psychology, 71, 63–73.

— Matell, M. S., Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research 21, 139–170.

— Matsuda, F. (1996). Duration, distance, and speed judgments of two moving objects by 4- to 11-year olds.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 286–311.

— Matthews, M. R. (2000). Time for science education: how teaching the history and philosophy of pendulum motion can contribute to science literacy. New York: Kluwer Academic.

Перейти на страницу:

Все книги серии Большая наука

Похожие книги