— Vitaterna, M. H., King, D. P., Chang, A.-M., Kornhauser, J. M., Lowrey, P. L., McDonald, J. D., Dove, W. F., Pinto, L. H., Turek, F. W., Takahashi, J. S. (1994). Mutagenesis and mapping of a mouse gene, clock, essential for circadian behavior.
— Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity.
— Wearden, J. H. (2015). Passage of time judgments.
— Wearden, J. H., Edwards, H., Fakhri, M., Percival, A. (1998). Why “sounds are judged longer than lights”: Application of a model of the internal clock in humans.
— Wearden, J. H., O’Donoghue, A., Ogden, R., Montgomery, C. (2014). Subjective duration in the laboratory and the world outside. In: Subjective time: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of temporality (Arstila, V, Lloyd, D, eds.), 287–306. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
— Weaver, D. R. (1998). The suprachiasmatic nucleus: A 25-year retrospective.
— Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
— Weiner, J. (1999). Time, love, memory: A great biologist and his quest for the origins of behavior. New York: Vintage Books.
— Wells, R. B. D. (1860). Illustrated hand-book of phrenology, physiology, and physiognomy. London: H. Vickers.
— Welsh, D., Engle, E. R. A., Richardson, G., Dement, W. (1986). Precision of circadian wake and activity onset timing in the mouse.
— Weyl, H. (1949/2009). Philosophy of mathematics and natural science. Princeton: Princeton University Press. В русском переводе: Вейль Г. О философии математики. — КомКнига, 2005.
— Whiting, A., Donthu N. (2009). Closing the gap between perceived and actual waiting times in a call center: Results from a field study.
— Wiener, M., Turkeltaub, P., Coslett, H. B. (2010). The image of time: A voxelwise meta-analysis.
— Wilson, M. A., McNaughton, B. L. (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep.
— Wise, S. P. (2008). Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function.
— Wiskott, L., Sejnowski, T. J. (2002). Slow feature analysis: unsupervised learning of invariances.
— Wittmann, M., Paulus, M. P. (2007). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception.
— Wood, J. N., Grafman, J. (2003). Human prefrontal cortex: processing and representational perspectives.
— Wright, B. A., Buonomano, D. V., Mahncke, H. W., Merzenich, M. M. (1997). Learning and generalization of auditory temporal-interval discrimination in humans.
— Wright, B. A., Wilson, R. M., Sabin, A. T. (2010). Generalization lags behind learning on an auditory perceptual task.
— Xuan, B., Zhang, D., He, S., Chen, X. (2007). Larger stimuli are judged to last longer.
— Yang, Y., Duguay, D., Bédard, N., Rachalski, A., Baquiran, G., Na, C. H., Fahrenkrug J., Storch, K.-F., Peng, J., Wing, S. S., Cermakian, N. (2012). Regulation of behavioral circadian rhythms and clock protein PER1 by the deubiquitinating enzyme USP2.
— Yarrow, K., Haggard, P., Heal, R., Brown, P., Rothwell, J. C. (2001). Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity.
— Zakay, D., Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition.
— Zantke, J., Ishikawa-Fujiwara, T., Arboleda, E., Lohs, C., Schipany, K., Hallay, N., Straw, A. D., Todo, T., Tessmar-Raible, K. (2013). Circadian and circalunar clock interactions in a marine annelid.
— Zarco, W., Merchant, H., Prado, L., Mendez, J. C. (2009). Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys.
— Zeh, H. D. (1989/2007). The physical basis of the direction of time. Berlin: Springer.