Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R.
(2012). The risks we dread: A social circle account. PLoS ONE, 7, e32837.Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R., Gigerenzer G.
(2009). Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks to low-numeracy people. Health Psychology 28,210–216.
Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R., Gigerenzer G.
(2014). Maximizing and satisficing: Personality trait or adaptive strategies? Manuscript, Max Planck Institute for Human Development: Berlin.Garcia-Retamero R., Galesic M.
(2012). Doc, what would you do if you were me? On self-other discrepancies in medical decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 18, 38–51.Gaskell G., Allansdottir A., Allum N., Corchero C., Fischler C., Hampel, J., et al.
(2006). Europeans and biotechnology in 2005: Pattern and trends. Eurobarometer 64.3.Gawande A.
(2009). The checklist manifesto. New York: Metropolitan Books.Ghosh A. K., Ghosh K.
(2005). Translating evidence-based information into effective risk communication: Current challenges and opportunities. Journal of Laboratory and Clinical Medicine 145, 171–180. doi:10.1016/ j.lab.2005.02.006.Gibson R., Singh J. P.
(2010). The treatment trap. Chicago, IL: Dee.Gigerenzer G.
(2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. New York: Oxford University Press.Gigerenzer G.
(2002). Calculated risks: How to know when numbers deceive you. New York: Simon and Schuster (UK version: Reckoning with risk: Learning to live with uncertainty. London: Penguin).Gigerenzer G.
(2004). Dread risk, September 11, and fatal traffic accidents. Psychological Science 15, 286–287 doi:10.1111/j.0956–7976.200 4.00668.x.Gigerenzer G.
(2006). Out of the frying pan into the fire: Behavioral reactions to terrorist attacks. Risk Analysis 26, 347–351. doi:10.1111/ j.1539–6924.2006.00753.x.Gigerenzer G.
(2007). Gut feelings: The intelligence of the unconscious. New York: Viking (UK version: London: Allen Lane/Penguin).Gigerenzer G.
(2008). Rationality for mortals: How people cope with uncertainty. New York: Oxford University Press.Gigerenzer G.
(2011). What are natural frequencies? Doctors need to find better ways to communicate risk to patients. British Medical Journal 343: d6386.Gigerenzer G., Brighton H.
(2009). Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science 1, 107–143.Gigerenzer G., Fiedler K., Olsson H.
(2012). Rethinking cognitive biases as environmental consequences. In P. M. Todd, G. Gigerenzer, the ABC Research Group, Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world (p. 80–110). New York: Oxford University Press.Gigerenzer G., Gaissmaier W., Kurz-Milcke E., Schwartz L. M., Woloshin S.
(2007). Helping doctors and patients to make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 8, 53–96.Gigerenzer G., Galesic M.
(2012). Why do single event probabilities confuse patients? British Medical Journal 344, e245. doi:0.1136/ bmj.e245.Gigerenzer G., Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R.
(2013). Stereotypes about men’s and women’s intuitions: A study of two nations. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology. doi:10.1177/0022022113487074.Gigerenzer G., Goldstein D. G.
(2011). The recognition heuristic: A decade of research. Judgment and Decision Making 6, 100–121.Gigerenzer G., Hertwig R., van den Broek E., Fasolo B., Katsikopoulos K. V.
(2005). «A 30 % chance of rain tomorrow»: How does the public understand probabilistic weather forecasts? Risk Analysis 25, 623–629.Gigerenzer G., Hertwig R., and Pachur T.
(Eds.) (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Gigerenzer G., Mata, J., Frank R.
(2009). Public knowledge of benefits of breast and prostate cancer screening in Europe. Journal of the National Cancer Institute 101(17), 1216–1220. doi:10.1093/jnci/djp237.Gigerenzer G., Muir Gray J. A.
(Eds.) (2011). Better doctors, better patients, better decisions: Envisioning health care 2020. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Gigerenzer G., Selten R.
(2001). Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.