Grainger, J., Granier, J. P., Farioli, F., Van Assche, E., & van Heuven, W. J. (2006). Letter position information and printed word perception: The relative-position priming constraint.
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model.
Grainger, J., & van Heuven, W. (2003). Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. // Bonin, P. (Ed.),
Grainger, J., & Whitney, C. (2004). Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe?
Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. (2007). Cross-code consistency effects in visual word recognition. // Grigorenko, E. L., & Naples, A. J. (Eds.),
Grill-Spector, K., Kushnir, T., Hendler, T., Edelman, S. Itzchak, Y., & Malach, R. (1998). A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe.
Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2001). fMR-adaptation: A tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons.
Grill-Spector, K., Sayres, R., & Ress, D. (2006). High-resolution imaging reveals highly selective nonface clusters in the fusiform face area.
Grigorenko, E. L. (2003). The first candidate gene for dyslexia: Turning the page of a new chapter of research.
Ha Duy Thuy, D., Matsuo, K., Nakamura, K., Toma, K., Oga, T., Nakai, T., Shibasaki, H., & Fukuyama, H. (2004). Implicit and explicit processing of Kanji and Kana words and non-words studied with fMRI.
Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: An overview and working hypothesis.
Hagoort, P., Indefrey, P., Brown, C., Herzog, H., Steinmetz, H., & Seitz R. J. (1999). The neural circuitry involved in the reading of German words and pseudowords: A PET study.
Hammond, P., & Hughes, P. (1978).
Hanley, J. R., & Kay, J. (1996). Reading speed in pure alexia.
Hannula-Jouppi, K., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Taipale, M., Eklund, R., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Kaariainen, H., & Kere, J. (2005). The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia.
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models.
–—. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes.
Hasson, U., Harel, M., Levy, I., & Malach, R. (2003). Large-scale mirror-symmetry organization of human occipito-temporal object areas.
Hasson, U., Levy, I., Behrmann, M., Hendler, T., & Malach, R. (2002). Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas.
Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., & Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex.
Haxby, J. V., Ishai, I. I., Chao, L. L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Martin, I. I. (2000). Object-form topology in the ventral temporal lobe: Response to I. Gauthier (2000).
Heilman, K. M., Howell, G., Valenstein, E., & Rothi, L. (1980). Mirror-reading and writing in association with right-left spatial disorientation.
Helenius, P., Tarkiainen, A., Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P. C., & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity and development: The case of spatial reorientation.