Читаем Shufflebrain полностью

I reasoned that an "active-negative" component must be operative in the salamander's intelligence. What Triclops didn't do was as important as what he did. But manifested as non-response, indistinguishable from no reaction, hidden on a completely invisible hyperplane[10], the active-negative mode would, of course, go undetected in conventional paradigm, which is what light-shock was. An attempt to determine intelligence from IQ is like being in a fight with an opponent who has an invisible arm. You never know when a thunderbolt will come flying out of Kant's realm of noumena to lay you flat. No, I concluded, the conventional tests don't measure intelligence. They measure IQ.

***

I also concluded that one-to-one is a valid principle and becomes useful providing we understand its epistemological nature. Like the active-negative mode, one-to-one is not simple arithmetic, but algebraic in character. And one-to-one is not an a priori principle of sensation-perception-learning. It is a by-product of intelligence. The mind imposes one-to-one, not the other way around. It is an effect not a cause, a consequence, not an antecedent.

I spent the next few days translating the main argument into scientese, drafted a manuscript and give it to Carl. He corrected my spelling, made a few minor changes and we shipped it off to Brain Research, which accepted it without changes, and published it in 1968. And Carl and I went on to other things.

***

What did Triclops's two normal eyes do for him? I ponder the question, even today. But it was several years before even a clue surfaced. The main obstacle was the test itself, which, after Triclops and Cyclops, looked to me like the sure route to dysinformation. But then I found a vision-dependent response that reopened the whole issue. As with the Looking-up reaction, the discovery was an outcome of making do.

I had some students working in the lab and we happened to have a larger number of larvae than of glass finger bowls. As a substitute, I bought polystyrene Dixie cups, which were inert and, by the gross, cost less than a half cent a piece. (I still owe my wife for them.) The cups were a brilliant bride's white. Against this background, the normal animals blanched to a very bright coloration, which they maintained, I found, even when the illumination was drastically curtailed (to moonlight levels). Transferred to a black pan, the normal animals darken until you can hardly find them; in clear cups or bowls, they assume a tawny color. In marked contrast, eyeless animals when illuminated assumed dark coloration in recepticles of any color, including the white cups.

Over the years, I'd become vaguely aware of the larva's ability to alter skin coloration, and I knew there was a literature on what is their camouflage reactions (the technical term is metachrosis) out in the wilds. It isn't anything like the chameleon, not dramatic or quick, and is not rwith the animals in clear crystal. But in the white cups, the reactions were conspicuous and they caught my immediate attention.

After reading all I could find in the scientific literature and conducting some preliminary experimentation to convince myself that normal camouflage reactions are visually elicited, I decided to look use test Cyclops for the reaction.[11]

I had to have some way of judging when the grafted eyes were functional and, I realized, and therefore set up a group of controls called Orthoclops.

Ortho means the same. In the Orthoclops, I removed and immediately replaced the natural eye. It took about a month for the Orthoclops to recovery the full range of camouflage reactions.

Cyclops? My expectation was that, soon after the Orthoclopes showed me a viable camouflage reaction, the Cyclopes would too. That didn't happen. Cyclops darkened in white cups.

Now if all the Cyclopes had simply acted like a typical eyeless animals, I might have been a little embarrassed, but I would have ruled the top-mounted eye incapable of carrying visually meaningful sense data and have written an article critical of our previous observations. And that would have been that.

But Cyclops didn't fully darken like eyeless--not all of them, anyway. And many in the Cyclops group responded positively to vision function tests. I even shipped some off to Carl for light-shock testing. The Cyclops not only saw but the comparative scores were identical to what we'd observed years before. I had no choice but to keep on going with the camouflage reaction.

Перейти на страницу:

Похожие книги

Фаина Раневская. Клочки воспоминаний
Фаина Раневская. Клочки воспоминаний

Фаина Георгиевна Раневская, урожденная Фельдман (1896–1984), — великая русская актриса. Трижды лауреат Сталинской премии, народная артистка СССР.«Я дочь небогатого нефтепромышленника из Таганрога» — так говорила о себе Раневская. Фуфа Великолепная — так называли ее друзья и близкие. Невероятно острой, даже злой на язык была великая актриса, она органически не переносила пошлости и мещанства в жизни, что уж говорить о театре, которому она фанатично служила всю жизнь.Фаина Раневская начинала писать воспоминания по заказу одного из советских издательств, но в итоге оставила это занятие, аргументируя свое решение следующим: «Деньги прожрешь, а стыд останется».В этой книге по крупицам собраны воспоминания о великой актрисе ее коллег и друзей, ее высказывания — ироничные и злые, грустные и лиричные, письма актрисы, адресатами которых были Анна Ахматова, Марина Цветаева, Осип Мандельштам.

Иван Андреев , Коллектив авторов , Фаина Георгиевна Раневская

История / Неотсортированное / Образование и наука / Документальное / Биографии и Мемуары