E. E. Birch, S. Shimojo, and R. Held, “Preferential-Looking Assessment of Fusion and Stereopsis in Infants Aged 1–6 Months,” Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science
26 (1985): 366–370; R. Fox et al., “Stereopsis in Human Infants,” Science 207 (1980): 323–324; B. Petrig et al., “Development of Stereopsis and Cortical Binocularity in Human Infants: Electrophysiological Evidence,” Science 213 (1981): 1402–1405; F. Thorn et al. “The Development of Eye Alignment, Convergence, and Sensory Binocularity in Young Infants,” Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 35 (1994): 544–553.
106
M. E. Arteberry and P. J. Kellman, Development of Perception in Infancy: The Cradle of Knowledge Revisited
(New York: Oxford University Press, 2016); M. Arterberry, A. Yonas, and A. S. Bensen, “Self-Produced Locomotion and the Development of Responsiveness to Linear Perspective and Texture Gradients,” Developmental Psychology 25 (1989): 976–982; M. Kavsek, A. Yonas, and C. E. Granrud, “Infants’ Sensitivity to Pictorial Depth Cues: A Review and Meta-analysis of Looking Studies,” Infant Behavior and Development 35 (2012): 109–128; A. Tsuruhara et al., “The Development of the Ability of Infants to Utilize Static Cues to Create and Access Representations of Object Shape,” Journal of Vision 10 (2010), doi:10.1167/10.12.2; A. Yonas and C. E. Granrud, “Infants’ Perception of Depth from Cast Shadows,” Perception & Psychophysics 68 (2006): 154–160.
107
H. Wallach and A. O’Leary, “Slope of Regard as a Distance Cue,” Perception & Psychophysics
31 (1982): 145–148; A. M. Norcia et al., “Experience-Expectant Development of Contour Integration Mechanisms in Human Visual Cortex,” Journal of Vision 5 (2005): 116–130.
108
M. von Senden, Space and Sight: The Perception of Space and Shape in the Congenitally Blind Before and After Operation
(Glencoe, IL: Free Press, 1960).
109
B. Tversky, Mind in Motion: How Action Shapes Thought
(New York: Basic Books, 2019).
110
S. Hochstein and M. Ahissar, “View from the Top: Hierarchies and Reverse Hierarchies in the Visual System,” Neuron
36 (2002): 791–804.
111
Von Senden, Space and Sight
; R. L. Gregory and J. G. Wallace, Recovery from Early Blindness: A Case Study, Monograph No. 2 (Cambridge, UK: Experimental Psychology Society, 1963); E. Huber et al., “A Lack of Experience-Dependent Plasticity After More Than a Decade of Recovered Sight,” Psychological Science 26 (2015): 393–401; O. Sacks, “To See and Not See,” in An Anthropologist on Mars: Seven Paradoxical Tales (New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1995); A. Valvo, Sight Restoration After Long-Term Blindness: The Problems and Behavior Patterns of Visual Rehabilitation (New York: American Federation for the Blind, 1971).
112
O. Sacks, “To See and Not See,” in An Anthropologist on Mars: Seven Paradoxical Tales
(New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1995).
113
R. Kurson, Crashing Through: A True Story of Risk, Adventure, and the Man Who Dared to See
(New York: Random House, 2007).
114
M. E. Arterberry and P. J. Kellman, Development of Perception in Infancy: The Cradle of Knowledge Revisited
(New York: Oxford University Press, 2016).
115
Arterberry and Kellman, Development of Perception in Infancy
; K. J. Kellman and E. S. Spelke, “Perception of Partly Occluded Objects in Infancy,” Cognitive Psychology 15 (1983): 483–524.
116
M. Wertheimer, “Laws of Organization in Perceptual Forms,” in A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology
, ed. W. Ellis (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1938), 71–88. Первая публикация: “Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt II,” Psycologische Forschung 4 (1923): 301–350.
117
B. Tversky, Mind in Motion: How Action Shapes Thought
(New York: Basic Books, 2019).
118
J. J. Gibson, The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception
(Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers, 1986).
119
A. Michotte, The Perception of Causality
(New York: Basic Books, 1963).
120